Article

What the Hell is the Deal with the Phantom Side Effects of Hormonal Birth Control Methods?

At the very pushy urging of the doctors at my college’s health center, I started the pill⁠ the first week of my freshman year. It made me feel terrible. I was emotionally unstable, sad, and disconnected from myself. Once, I cried hysterically in the cafeteria because I waited in line for some chicken but the person before me got the last piece. That moment was a relief actually because it gave me an objective measure of my emotional reactions: something was off. I know with absolute certainty that I don’t love chicken that much. But when I went back to the doctors, they twice assured me that it was just freshman year of college—not the pill—making me feel that way. In fact, I specifically remember them saying, “This is the most studied drug on the planet. There are no side effects.”

I don’t know if I started to feel more normal, or if I just got used to feeling terrible. My memories of college are generally bad, so it may be the latter.

When I finally went off the pill, I went through another huge emotional upheaval, didn’t get my period⁠ for about six months, and started growing thick, black hair on the backs of my knees and tops of my toes. I’ve never gone back on any form of hormonal contraception⁠ because fuck. that. The whole experience made me feel gaslit by my doctors and scared of intervening with my body.

This is not meant to be a cautionary tale.

Many, many, many people have a much easier and totally fine time with birth control⁠ pills and other forms of hormonal contraception (HC). Indeed, many people love HC. And while no other life change has made me feel that terrible, it’s entirely possible that the emotional instability actually was a symptom of starting college. But also, I’m far from being the only one who’s experienced side effects like these. Anecdotal evidence of people feeling terrible on HC is everywhere. There was even a viral New York Times video about a person feeling terrible on HC in 2018, called Birth Control Your Own Adventure by Sindha Agha (and it’s awesome).

Also, the idea that HC could cause depression is totally believable. First of all, sex⁠ hormones⁠ , like the ones in HC, affect a person’s mood in several ways. This is well accepted and has been demonstrated over and over by many studies.1,2,3 Second, back when HC had way higher doses of hormones than currently available forms do, studies showed that depression was a fairly common side effect (increases in depressive symptoms ranged from 20% to 50%).1 Third of all, many studies have shown that mood disruption and mental illness symptoms are still major reasons that people give for why they stop taking birth control.4,5,6,7,8

But the research on birth control and depression tells an inconsistent story.

There are thousands of studies on the subject, but a few are more highly regarded than the rest because of how they collected and analyzed their data. Four randomized controlled studies (the best, most trustworthy kind of study because it curbs biases in results and uses a control group for comparison, which many studies lack!) looked at whether or not HC causes mood changes, and none of them made a strong case that it does.9,10,11,12 In fact, one found that HC improved symptoms of depression. And then there are the studies that were not randomized and controlled but that made up for that in scale (a lot of study-sample biases disappear when the group being studied is big enough). One Australian study followed 10,000 people with pussies for three years. Another American study looked at 7,000 people with pussies over the course of 14 years. Still nothing linking HC to mood changes. Both showed a slight decrease in depressive symptoms over time.13,14

Illustration of birth control methods by María Conejo from PussypediaOne huge Danish study that followed 1,061,997 people with pussies from 2001 to 2013 did show that HC use was associated with an increase in the likelihood that someone would start taking antidepressants or get diagnosed with depression at a psychiatric hospital (2.2 and 0.3 per 100 person-years,* respectively, compared with 1.7 and 0.28 per 100 person-years for nonusers), especially among adolescents. Interestingly, it found that the risk of depression increased steadily for the first six months of HC use, then decreased slowly, returning to pre-use levels four years later. Also, rates of depression were higher for users of progestin-only methods than they were for users of combined methods.3

A 2019 review of 90 smaller-scale studies on the effects of HC identified a few consistent patterns of HC side effects such as: a decreased ability to read other people’s emotions as well as thinking everyone else is feeling worse than they really are; having way more intense emotional reactions (crying about the chicken); getting less pleasure out⁠ of things we like; and messing up the way we respond to stress. These feel like validating findings, but the data isn’t, like, superstrong since the studies reviewed are all fairly new and most haven’t been replicated. (As the authors remind us, “Results have to be interpreted with caution, as the evidence is observational in most studies, and therefore, we cannot infer causality.”)15,16

And still, it’s a fucking lot of studies. What this review made me think is that perhaps a lot of studies that look at HC’s effects are too focused on depression when maybe what we’re looking for is just a nebulous combination of smaller, harder-to-pinpoint effects that amount to feeling weird and bad.

Other recent reviews of studies about hormonal birth control have tried both to explain why so many studies have turned up inconsistent results and to identify any consistencies among studies’ results.

Their conclusions were:1,2

  1. There aren’t enough studies that look at people using HC over a long period of time.
  2. Too many studies lumped together people taking different kinds of hormonal birth control and then studied them as a whole rather than looking at the effects of specific methods or doses.
  3. Studies define and measure mood changes differently.
  4. People’s expectations of HC’s mood effects might be throwing off the data. If people expect to have mood changes (one study found that 20% of subjects did), and then they don’t, they might report their mood as extra good. Also, people with the idea that they might feel shitty on HC might end up feeling shitty just because they expected to feel shitty.
  5. A critical mass of studies show that adolescents, people who have a history of depression, and people with previous bad experiences with HC are all at a higher risk of developing depression on HC.

Dr. Ellen Støkken Dahl and Dr. Nina Brochmann also reviewed this mystery and some of these studies in [the book] The Wonder Down Under. They concluded that this whole thing might just come down to coincidence and confirmation bias:

Depression, mood changes, and irritability are such common phenomena in the population that this is likely to be a trick of chance. If, in addition, you’ve heard that contraceptive pills can cause mood changes and depression, it’s even more likely you’ll draw this conclusion, given that [many people report negative effects from placebo treatments if they think those treatments cause negative effects]. Rumors of mood changes spread like wildfire among female friends on Internet forums, and suddenly, you start to see your own experiences in a new light.17

They might be right. Their theory is plausible. But based on the data, my personal experience, and the countless conversations I’ve had with so many people about their similar experiences, I have a feeling that there’s more to be discovered. If you’re crying about chicken, I don’t think you’re hysterical. But for more of an explanation, we’ll have to wait and see.

*Person-years is a measurement that combines the number of people in the study with the number of years they were studied. This measurement is used because the number of people in a study over time can change (like if people drop out). A study that looks at 10 people for 10 years would have 100 person-years of data.


Excerpted from Pussypedia: A Comprehensive Guide by Zoe Mendelson, illustrated by María Conejo, inclusing the illustration shown on this page. Copyright © 2021. Available from Hachette Go, an imprint of Hachette Book Group, Inc.


SOURCES

  1. Schaffir, Johnathan, Brett L. Worly, and Tamar L. Gur. “Combined Hormonal Contraception and Its Effects on Mood: A Critical Review. European Journal of Contraception & Reproductive Health Care 21, no. 5 (August 2016): 347–55.
  2. Fruzzetti, Franca, and Tiziana Fidecicchi. “Hormonal Contraception and Depression: Updated Evidence and Implications in Clinical Practice.Clinical Drug Investigation 40 (September 2020): 1097–106.
  3. Skovlund, Wessel Charlotte, Lina Steinrud Mørch, Lars Vedel Kessing, et al. “Association of Hormonal Contraception with Depression. JAMA Psychiatry 73, no. 11 (November 2016): 1154–62.
  4. Larsson, Gerd, Febe Blohm, Gunilla Sundell, et al. “A Longitudinal Study of Birth Control and Pregnancy Outcome Among Women in a Swedish Population.Contraception 56, no. 1 (July 1997): 9–16.
  5. Rosenberg, Michael J., and Michael S. Waugh. “Oral Contraceptive Discontinuation: A Prospective Evaluation of Frequency and Reasons.American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 179, no. 3 (September 1998): 577–82.
  6. Sanders, Stephanie A., Cynthia A. Graham, Jennifer L. Bass, et al. “A Prospective Study of the Effects of Oral Contraceptives on Sexuality and Well-Being and Their Relationship to Discontinuation.Contraception 64, no. 1 (July 2001): 51–58.
  7. Poromaa, Inger Sundström, and Birgitta Segebladh. “Adverse Mood Symptoms with Oral Contraceptives.Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica 91, no. 4 (December 2011): 420–27.
  8. Rapkin, Andrea J., Giovanni Biggio, and Alessandra Concas. “Oral Contraceptives and Neuroactive Steroids.Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 84, no. 4 (August 2006): 628–34.
  9. O’Connell, Katharine, Anne R. Davis, and Jennifer Kerns. “Oral Contraceptives: Side Effects and Depression in Adolescent Girls.Contraception 75, no. 4 (April 2007): 299–304.
  10. Redmond, Geoffrey, Amy J. Godwin, William Olson, et al. “Use of Placebo Controls in an Oral Contraceptive Trial: Methodological Issues and Adverse Event and Incidence.Contraception 60, no. 2 (August 1999): 81–85.
  11. Graham, Cynthia A., and Barbara B. Sherwin. “The Relationship Between Mood and Sexuality in Women Using an Oral Contraceptive as Treatment for Premenstrual Symptoms.Psychoneuroendocrinology 18, no. 4 (January 1993): 273–81.
  12. Graham, Cynthia, Rebecca Ramos, John Bancroft, et al. “The Effects of Steroid Contraceptives on the Well-Being and Sexuality of Women: A Double Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Two-Centre Study of Combined and Progestogen-Only Methods.Contraception 52, no. 6 (December 1995): 363–69.
  13. Duke, Janine, David W. Sibbritt, and Anne F. Young. “Is There an Association Between the Use of Oral Contraception and Depressive Symptoms in Young Australian Women?Contraception 75, no. 1 (January 2007): 27–31.
  14. Keyes, Katherine M., Keely Cheslack-Postava, Carolyn Westhoff, et al. “Association of Hormonal Contraceptive Use with Reduced Levels of Depression Symptoms: National Study of Sexually Active Women in the United States.American Journal of Epidemiology 178, no. 9 (November 2013): 1378–88.
  15. Lewis, Carolin, Ann-Christin S. Kimmig, Rachel G. Zsido, et al. “Effects of Hormonal Contraceptives on Mood: A Focus on Emotion Recognition and Reactivity, Reward Processing, and Stress Response.Current Psychiatry Reports 21, no. 115 (November 2019): P27–31.
  16. Wimberly, Yolanda H., Sian Cotton, Abbey M. Wanchick, et al. “Attitudes and Experiences with Levonorgestrel 100 μg/ethinyl estradiol 20 μg Among Women During a 3-Month Trial.Contraception 65, no. 6 (July 2002): 403–6.
  17. Brochmann, Nina, and Ellen Støkken Dahl. The Wonder Down Under. New York: Quercus, 2017.

Similar articles and advice

Article
  • Sam Wall

An article about choosing methods and trusting methods